Click
here to close Hello! We notice that
you are using Internet Explorer, which is not supported by Echinobase
and may cause the site to display incorrectly. We suggest using a
current version of Chrome,
FireFox,
or Safari.
Decision making improves sperm chemotaxis in the presence of noise.
Kromer JA
,
Märcker S
,
Lange S
,
Baier C
,
Friedrich BM
.
???displayArticle.abstract???
To navigate their surroundings, cells rely on sensory input that is corrupted by noise. In cells performing chemotaxis, such noise arises from the stochastic binding of signalling molecules at low chemoattractant concentrations. We reveal a fundamental relationship between the speed of chemotactic steering and the strength of directional fluctuations that result from the amplification of noise in a chemical input signal. This relation implies a trade-off between steering that is slow and reliable, and steering that is fast but less reliable. We show that dynamic switching between these two modes of steering can substantially increase the probability to find a target, such as an egg to be found by sperm cells. This decision making confers no advantage in the absence of noise, but is beneficial when chemical signals are detectable, yet characterized by low signal-to-noise ratios. The latter applies at intermediate distances from a target, where signalling molecules are diluted, thus defining a ''noise zone'' that cells have to cross. Our results explain decision making observed in recent experiments on sea urchin sperm chemotaxis. More generally, our theory demonstrates how decision making enables chemotactic agents to cope with high levels of noise in gradient sensing by dynamically adjusting the persistence length of a biased random walk.
Fig 1. Decision making in chemotaxis of sea urchin sperm.(A) Helical swimming path of a sea urchin sperm cell (black) with helix centreline (red), while navigating in a concentration field of the chemoattractant resact [16]. The concentration field is cylindrically symmetric with symmetry axis parallel to the z-axis (indicated in blue). (B) Projection of the same swimming path on the xy-plane. Dots mark the beginning (black) and peak (red) of âhigh-gainâ steering phases (or off-responses [16]). The concentration field is indicated by blue circles. (C) From the swimming path and the local gradient direction, we can determine a time-dependent rate γ(t) of helix bending towards the gradient [16]. The beginning of a âhigh-gainâ steering phase is defined as the level-crossing of γ(t) above its median as is indicated by black dots. Peaks of γ(t) are indicated by red dots. (D) Scatter plot of the orientation angle Ψ and local concentration c at the beginning of âhigh-gainâ steering phases (n = 9 cells). âHigh-gainâ steering is predominantly initiated for Ψ > Ï/2 (grey shading).
Fig 2. Helical chemotaxis in the presence of sensing noise.(A) Chemoattractant molecules bind to receptors on the cell membrane. (B) The sequence of binding events defines a stochastic input signal s(t) with rate b(t), Eq 1. (C) A sperm cell swims along a helical swimming path (black), whose centreline (red) can bend in the direction of a concentration gradient (blue). (D) Helical swimming in a concentration gradient causes a periodic modulation of the rate b(t) of binding events (red). Representative realization of input signal s(t) (black, low-pass filtered for visualization). This signal dynamically regulates the path curvature κ(t), here shown in the absence of sensing noise (red) and for stochastic input signal (black). (E) Example swimming paths with and without sensing noise for two values of the gain factor (âlow-gainâ steering Ïlow = 1, âhigh-gain steeringâ Ïhigh = 10). Egg cell (yellow disk). (F) Signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) as a function of distance R from the egg. The SNR defines a ânoise zoneâ spanning intermediate distances R, bounded by a noise zone boundary N, where SNR = 1, and a spatial limit of chemosensation S, where c(R) = (λT)â1. (G) Probability to find the egg as a function of gain factor Ï for initial distance R0 = 3 mm to the egg (and random initial orientation). Without sensing noise, the success probability increases monotonically with Ï, while in the presence of noise, this probability displays a maximum at an optimal Ï. Maximum search time 300s. Error bars smaller than symbols. Parameters chosen to match experiment, see S1 Appendix.
Fig 3. Sperm navigation mapped on a Markov decision process.(A,B) Binning of (R, Ψ)-phase space and sketch of trajectories for âlow-gainâ (white) and âhigh-gainâ (black) steering. (C) Illustration of a single decision: Starting in a state 1, the player first chooses between two actions, i.e. âlow-gainâ steering or âhigh-gainâ steering. This choice determines the transition probabilities Lij for jumping to a different state, here labelled 2 and 3. (D) Illustration of a memoryless decision strategy, assigning a choice of action to each state. The figure shows coarse bins for sake of illustration.
Fig 4. Chemotactic success with decision making.Success probability P(R0) for the optimal decision strategy, resulting from switching between âlow-gainâ and âhigh-gainâ steering, as function of initial distance R0 to the egg for the case of noise-free concentration measurements (A), and physiological levels of sensing noise (B) (red squares). For comparison, success probabilities for strategies without decision making are shown (circles). (C,D) Optimal decision strategies for the cases shown in panel A and B. Greyscale represents prediction frequency of âhigh-gainâ steering, using a cohort of MDPs obtained by bootstrapping, see S1 Appendix for details. Arrows and dashed lines indicate zone boundaries as introduced in Fig 2. (E,F) Spatial sensitivity analysis of optimal strategies: Shown is the change in chemotactic range R as function of cut-off distance Rc for hybrid strategies that employ the optimal strategy for R < Rc, and either âlow-gainâ steering (white circles) or âhigh-gainâ steering (black circles) else. Positive values indicate a benefit of decision making at the respective distance to the egg. Parameters, see S1 Appendix.
Fig 5. Simple implementation of optimal decision making.(A) Signalling variables p and q contain information about the helix orientation angle Ψ and distance R to the target. Contour levels for conditional probability densities P(p,q|R,Ψ>Ï2) (red) and P(p,q|R,Ψâ¤Ï2) (black) (corresponding to 1%, 10%, 50%, 90% percentiles; R = 1.5mm). (B) Relative frequency of âhigh-gainâ steering predicted by the optimal decision strategy, for given combination of (p, q). We define a decision boundary Î(p) (yellow) by a piecewise linear fit to the 50%-contour line (up to p = 5ms, corresponding to a limit of sufficiently reliable state estimation, see S1 Appendix). (C) Simulated swimming path using this decision rule with dynamic switching between âhigh-gainâ steering (red) and âlow-gainâ steering (black); projected on xy-plane. The chemoattractant concentration in this plane is shown (blue gradient), together with the boundary of the noise zone. (D) Success probability P(R0) for full simulations with simple decision making (red) as a function of initial distance R0 to the egg. For comparison, success probabilities for âlow-gainâ steering (white) and âhigh-gainâ steering (black) are shown. (E) The effective chemotactic range R with decision making (red) is larger than R for an optimal constant gain factor (black). Parameters, see S1 Appendix.
Alvarez,
The computational sperm cell.
2014, Pubmed
Alvarez,
The computational sperm cell.
2014,
Pubmed
Amselem,
Control parameter description of eukaryotic chemotaxis.
2012,
Pubmed
Andrews,
Optimal noise filtering in the chemotactic response of Escherichia coli.
2006,
Pubmed
Berg,
Physics of chemoreception.
1977,
Pubmed
Bialek,
Physical limits to biochemical signaling.
2005,
Pubmed
Celani,
Bacterial strategies for chemotaxis response.
2010,
Pubmed
Devreotes,
Chemotaxis in eukaryotic cells: a focus on leukocytes and Dictyostelium.
1988,
Pubmed
Eisenbach,
Sperm guidance in mammals - an unpaved road to the egg.
2006,
Pubmed
Endres,
Accuracy of direct gradient sensing by single cells.
2008,
Pubmed
Farley,
The role of jelly coats in sperm-egg encounters, fertilization success, and selection on egg size in broadcast spawners.
2001,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Friedrich,
Steering chiral swimmers along noisy helical paths.
2009,
Pubmed
Friedrich,
Chemotaxis of sperm cells.
2007,
Pubmed
Friedrich,
Search along persistent random walks.
2008,
Pubmed
Gomez-Marin,
Active sampling and decision making in Drosophila chemotaxis.
2011,
Pubmed
Hein,
Physical limits on bacterial navigation in dynamic environments.
2016,
Pubmed
Hein,
Sensing and decision-making in random search.
2012,
Pubmed
Jikeli,
Sperm navigation along helical paths in 3D chemoattractant landscapes.
2015,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Kashikar,
Temporal sampling, resetting, and adaptation orchestrate gradient sensing in sperm.
2012,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Loverdo,
Robustness of optimal intermittent search strategies in one, two, and three dimensions.
2009,
Pubmed
Pichlo,
High density and ligand affinity confer ultrasensitive signal detection by a guanylyl cyclase chemoreceptor.
2014,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Sourjik,
Responding to chemical gradients: bacterial chemotaxis.
2012,
Pubmed
Vergassola,
'Infotaxis' as a strategy for searching without gradients.
2007,
Pubmed
Viswanathan,
Optimizing the success of random searches.
1999,
Pubmed
Witman,
Chlamydomonas phototaxis.
1993,
Pubmed