Click
here to close Hello! We notice that
you are using Internet Explorer, which is not supported by Echinobase
and may cause the site to display incorrectly. We suggest using a
current version of Chrome,
FireFox,
or Safari.
PLoS One
2013 Jan 01;89:e73477. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073477.
Show Gene links
Show Anatomy links
Effects of a range-expanding sea urchin on behaviour of commercially fished abalone.
Strain EM
,
Johnson CR
,
Thomson RJ
.
???displayArticle.abstract???
BACKGROUND: Global climate change has resulted in a southerly range expansion of the habitat modifying sea urchin Centrostephanus rodgersii to the east coast of Tasmania, Australia. Various studies have suggested that this urchin outcompetes black-lipped abalone (Haliotis rubra) for resources, but experiments elucidating the mechanisms are lacking.
METHODOLOGY/PRINCIPAL FINDINGS: We outline a new framework involving experimental manipulations and Markov chain and Pareto modelling to examine the effects of interspecific competition between urchins and abalone and the effect of intraspecific competition in abalone, assessed as effects on behaviour. Manipulations of abalone densities had no detectable effect on urchin behavioural transitions, movement patterns or resightability through time. In contrast, additions of urchins resulted in abalone shifting microhabitats from exposed to sheltered positions, an increase in the proportion of mobile abalone, and declines in abalone resightability through time relative to controls without the urchins. Our results support the hypothesis of asymmetrical competitive interactions between urchins and abalone.
CONCLUSIONS/SIGNIFICANCE: The introduction of urchins to intact algal beds causes abalone to flee and seek shelter in cryptic microhabitat which will negatively impact both their accessibility to such microhabitats, and productivity of the abalone fishery, and will potentially affect their growth and survival, while the presence of the abalone has no detectable effect on the urchin. Our approach involving field-based experiments and modelling could be used to test the effects of other invasive species on native species behaviour.
???displayArticle.pubmedLink???
24073195
???displayArticle.pmcLink???PMC3779227 ???displayArticle.link???PLoS One
Figure 1. Effect of interspecific competition on the percentage of tagged abalone resighted through time (weeks), in Experiment 2, at Magistrates Point, Maria Island.Data are means (+/âSE) of nâ=â3 replicate plots. Squares are 0U18A â=â1Ã ambient density abalone (weeks 1â9) and circles are 18U25A â=â1Ã ambient density abalone with 1Ã ambient density urchins (weeks 1â3 were prior to adding urchins, weeks 4â6 with added urchins and weeks 7â9 after the urchins were removed). There were significant differences between 0U25A (weeks 1â3) vs. 18U25A (weeks 4â6), 18U1A (weeks 1â3) vs. 18U25A (weeks 7â9), (see Table 2).
Figure 2. Effects of interspecific competition on abalone behavioural transitions, in Experiment 2 at Magistrates Point, Maria Island.Data are the proportion of tagged abalone observed in each behavioural state the week before (Eâ=â exposed, Lâ=â lost, Oâ=â outside the plot and Sâ=â sheltered) in 3 replicate plots. (a) 0U25A: 1Ã ambient density abalone, (weeks 1â3 prior to adding urchins and weeks 7â9 after urchins were removed), (b) 18U25A: 1Ã ambient density urchins, with 1Ã ambient density abalone, (week 4 with added urchins), (c) 18U25A: 1Ã ambient density urchins with 1Ã ambient density abalone, (week 5 with added urchins), and (d) 18U25A: 1Ã ambient density urchins Ã1 ambient density abalone, (week 6 with added urchins). The numbers of abalone in a given behavioural state are summed across all times for the control and given separately at each time for the treatment and are represented as ânâ=â.â inside each circle. Arrows of different thickness are used to show the relative probabilities of abalone transitioning from each behavioural state. The straight arrows show the probabilities of abalone transitioning from one behavioural state to another (e.g. E to S) and the curved arrows show the probabilities of abalone âtransitioningâ to the same behavioural state (e.g. E to E). These proportions sum to 1 for a given behaviour state (e.g. E-E, E-S, E-L, E-O).
Figure 3. Effect of intraspecific competition on abalone behavioural transitions, in Experiment 2, at Magistrates Point, Maria Island.Data are the proportion of tagged abalone observed in each behavioural state the week before (Eâ=â exposed, Lâ=â lost, Oâ=â outside the plot and Sâ=â sheltered) in nâ=â3 replicate plots. (a) 0U18A: 1Ã ambient density abalone, (weeks 1â3 prior to adding extra abalone and weeks 7â9 after the extra abalone were removed combined), (b) 0U50A: 2Ã ambient density of abalone (week 4 with added extra abalone), (c) 0U50A: 2Ã ambient density of abalone, (week 5 with added extra abalone), and (d) 0U50A: 2Ã ambient density of abalone (week 6 with added extra abalone). The numbers of abalone in a given behavioural state are summed across all times for the control and given separately at each time for the treatment and are represented as ânâ=â.â inside each circle. Arrows of different thickness are used to show the relative probabilities of abalone transitioning from each behavioural state. The straight arrows show the probabilities of abalone transitioning from one behavioural state to another (e.g. E to S) and the curved arrows show the probabilities of abalone âtransitioningâ to the same behavioural (e.g. E to E). These proportions sum to 1 for a given behaviour state (e.g. E-E, E-S, E-L, E-O).
Figure 4. Effect of intra- and interspecific competition on the percentage of sedentary (â¤0.4 m) and mobile (>0.4 m) abalone through time (weeks) in Experiment 2 at Magistrates Point, Maria Island.(a) 0U25A: 1à ambient density of abalone with no urchins, (b) 18U25A: 1à ambient density of urchins with 1à ambient density of abalone, and (c) 0U50A: 2à ambient density of abalone with no urchins. Black bars are sedentary abalone (distance moved â¤0.4 m) and white bars are mobile abalone (net distances moved >0.4 m). There were significant differences between 0U18A vs. 0U50A in weeks 5 and 6 and 0U25A vs. 18U25A in weeks 4, 5 and 6 (see Table 3 results).
Creese,
Analysis of inter- and intra-specific competition amongst intertidal limpets with different methods of feeding.
1982, Pubmed
Creese,
Analysis of inter- and intra-specific competition amongst intertidal limpets with different methods of feeding.
1982,
Pubmed
Edwards,
Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer.
2007,
Pubmed
Firth,
Competition and habitat suitability: small-scale segregation underpins large-scale coexistence of key species on temperate rocky shores.
2010,
Pubmed
Ling,
Range expansion of a habitat-modifying species leads to loss of taxonomic diversity: a new and impoverished reef state.
2008,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
O'Connor,
Warming and resource availability shift food web structure and metabolism.
2009,
Pubmed
Parmesan,
A globally coherent fingerprint of climate change impacts across natural systems.
2003,
Pubmed
Poloczanska,
MODELING THE RESPONSE OF POPULATIONS OF COMPETING SPECIES TO CLIMATE CHANGE.
2008,
Pubmed
Walther,
Community and ecosystem responses to recent climate change.
2010,
Pubmed