Click
here to close Hello! We notice that
you are using Internet Explorer, which is not supported by Echinobase
and may cause the site to display incorrectly. We suggest using a
current version of Chrome,
FireFox,
or Safari.
Front Zool
2016 Sep 06;131:41. doi: 10.1186/s12983-016-0174-9.
Show Gene links
Show Anatomy links
Crown-of-thorns starfish have true image forming vision.
Petie R
,
Garm A
,
Hall MR
.
???displayArticle.abstract???
BACKGROUND: Photoreceptors have evolved numerous times giving organisms the ability to detect light and respond to specific visual stimuli. Studies into the visual abilities of the Asteroidea (Echinodermata) have recently shown that species within this class have a more developed visual sense than previously thought and it has been demonstrated that starfish use visual information for orientation within their habitat. Whereas image forming eyes have been suggested for starfish, direct experimental proof of true spatial vision has not yet been obtained.
RESULTS: The behavioural response of the coral reef inhabiting crown-of-thorns starfish (Acanthaster planci) was tested in controlled aquarium experiments using an array of stimuli to examine their visual performance. We presented starfish with various black-and-white shapes against a mid-intensity grey background, designed such that the animals would need to possess true spatial vision to detect these shapes. Starfish responded to black-and-white rectangles, but no directional response was found to black-and-white circles, despite equal areas of black and white. Additionally, we confirmed that starfish were attracted to black circles on a white background when the visual angle is larger than 14°. When changing the grey tone of the largest circle from black to white, we found responses to contrasts of 0.5 and up. The starfish were attracted to the dark area''s of the visual stimuli and were found to be both attracted and repelled by the visual targets.
CONCLUSIONS: For crown-of-thorns starfish, visual cues are essential for close range orientation towards objects, such as coral boulders, in the wild. These visually guided behaviours can be replicated in aquarium conditions. Our observation that crown-of-thorns starfish respond to black-and-white shapes on a mid-intensity grey background is the first direct proof of true spatial vision in starfish and in the phylum Echinodermata.
Fig. 1. Behavioural arena. a Schematic representation of the behavioural arena. Visual stimuli were attached to a Plexiglas sheet (indicated by hatching) using Velcro, with the bottom of the stimulus on the floor of the arena. The sheet was lowered into the arena and fastened by clamps. The wall of the arena was white. For the experiments with black-and-white patterns, a mid-intensity grey cloth was attached to the inside wall of the arena. b Five different stimuli were presented to the animals: three black-and-white stimuli, a black circular stimulus and a grey circular stimulus. For each black-and-white stimulus the area of black was equal to the area of white. For all similar sized circular, or rectangular, stimuli the area of black was the equal. c The arena during an experiment. Recordings were made with a camera floating on the surface of the water. Abbreviations: c, camera; cl, clamps for attaching the Plexiglas sheet; m, middle of the arena; s, stimulus; sf, starfish. Example tracks for: d black circles (angular height 37°) on a white background and e the control experiment with only the Plexiglas sheet on a white background. The stimulus is located at 0°
Fig. 2. Behavioural results: paired black and white rectangles presented on the mid-intensity grey background. a-e Animals were attracted to stimulus when the angular height of the stimuli was 32° or larger. The stimuli were always presented with the black rectangle to the left. The mean vector of the significant responses is pointing left of 0°, indicating that on average the animals were headed to the black half of the stimulus. In the circular plots, the small, black, filled circles represent the final angular positions of the animals. The direction of the arrow indicates the mean direction and the length of the arrow represents the length of the mean vector (rho). The radius of the circle represents a vector length of 1. The 95 % confidence intervals for the response are indicated by dashed lines, only when the p-value for the Rayleigh test is smaller than 0.05. A summary of the circular statistics is given in Table 1
Fig. 3. Behavioural results: black rectangle centred on a white square, presented on the mid-intensity grey background. a-e Starfish were either attracted to this stimulus or repelled, as seen by the axial nature of the directional responses. Animals responded to rectangles with an angular height of 5°, 32° and 43°. For more details on the circular plots see caption of Fig. 2. A summary of the circular statistics is given in Table 1
Fig. 4. Behavioural results: black-and-white circles on a mid-intensity grey background. a-f Animals did not show directional responses to the circles or the control experiment, where the Plexiglas sheet was presented against the grey background. The angular height of the black centre of the stimulus is given first, followed by the angular height of the entire stimulus. For more details on the circular plots see caption of Fig. 2. A summary of the circular statistics is given in Table 1
Fig. 5. Crown-of-thorns starfish perception of the black-and-white circles at varying distances. a Visual representation of the stimulus and the field of view of a single eye at varying distance between the starfish and the stimulus. The numbers indicate the distance of the eye to the stimulus in centimetres and the red lines mark the outline of the visual field. The stimulus circle shown here has an angular height of 37° seen from the centre of the arena. b Relation between the proportion of white in the field of view and the distance to the stimulus. Note that at close distance to the stimulus, the white area sampled by the eye was twice the size of the sampled black area
Fig. 6. Behavioural results: black circle on a white background. a-e Starfish were attracted to circles with initial angular sizes of 14° or larger. All stimuli were attached to a Plexiglas sheet using Velcro, and the entire sheet was placed in the behavioural arena. f Starfish where not attracted to the control stimulus, the Plexiglas sheet alone, without stimulus patterns. For more details on the circular plots see caption of Fig. 2. A summary of the circular statistics is given in Table 1
Fig. 7. Contrasts between corals and the surrounding water within the coral reef environment. Contrasts were measured on images taken through a blue filter with a transmittance curve similar to the spectral sensitivity curve of the starfish eye. The contrast at 1Â m measured 0.43 for the coral boulder and 0.18 for the blue staghorn coral. Two measurements were done on the same small reef, the first with the sun in the back and the second facing the sun
Fig. 8. Behavioural results: grey circle on a white background. a-e Animals responded to circles with an initial angular size of 37° when the contrast was 0.5 or higher. For more details on the circular plots see caption of Fig. 2. A summary of the circular statistics is given in Table 1
Aizenberg,
Calcitic microlenses as part of the photoreceptor system in brittlestars.
2001, Pubmed,
Echinobase
Aizenberg,
Calcitic microlenses as part of the photoreceptor system in brittlestars.
2001,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Atwood,
LARVAL DEVELOPMENT IN THE ASTEROID ECHINASTER ECHINOPHORUS.
1973,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Barker,
Sensorimotor decision making in the zebrafish tectum.
2015,
Pubmed
Beach,
Spawning pheromone in crown-of-thorns starfish.
1975,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Beer,
Active control of the visual field in the starfish Acanthaster planci.
2016,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Blevins,
Spatial vision in the echinoid genus Echinometra.
2004,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
De'ath,
The 27-year decline of coral cover on the Great Barrier Reef and its causes.
2012,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Delroisse,
De Novo Adult Transcriptomes of Two European Brittle Stars: Spotlight on Opsin-Based Photoreception.
2016,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Ewert,
Neural mechanisms of prey-catching and avoidance behavior in the toad (Bufo bufo L.).
1970,
Pubmed
Garm,
Visual navigation in starfish: first evidence for the use of vision and eyes in starfish.
2014,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Gorzelak,
Microlens arrays in the complex visual system of Cretaceous echinoderms.
2014,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Lesser,
Sea urchin tube feet are photosensory organs that express a rhabdomeric-like opsin and PAX6.
2011,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Lythgoe,
Visual pigments and visual range underwater.
1968,
Pubmed
Mäthger,
Color blindness and contrast perception in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) determined by a visual sensorimotor assay.
2006,
Pubmed
McKeon,
Species and size diversity in protective services offered by coral guard-crabs.
2014,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Morris,
Development of the five primary podia from the coeloms of a sea star larva: homology with the echinoid echinoderms and other deuterostomes.
2009,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Nilsson,
Eye evolution and its functional basis.
2013,
Pubmed
Nordström,
A simple visual system without neurons in jellyfish larvae.
2003,
Pubmed
Osborne,
Disturbance and the dynamics of coral cover on the Great Barrier Reef (1995-2009).
2011,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
ROCKSTEIN,
Role of the terminal pigment spots of the starfish, Asterias forbesi, in light orientation.
1956,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Ullrich-Lüter,
Unique system of photoreceptors in sea urchin tube feet.
2011,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Ullrich-Lüter,
C-opsin expressing photoreceptors in echinoderms.
2013,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Yerramilli,
Spatial vision in the purple sea urchin Strongylocentrotus purpuratus (Echinoidea).
2010,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase
Yoshida,
Compound ocellus of a starfish: its function.
1966,
Pubmed
,
Echinobase